An effortless way to improve your memory
A surprisingly potent technique can
boost your short and long-term recall – and it appears to help everyone from
students to Alzheimer’s patients.
By David Robson
12 February 2018
When trying to memorise new
material, it’s easy to assume that the more work you put in, the better you
will perform. Yet taking the occasional down time – to do literally nothing –
may be exactly what you need. Just dim the lights, sit
back, and enjoy 10-15 minutes of quiet contemplation, and you’ll find that your
memory of the facts you have just learnt is far better than if you had
attempted to use that moment more productively.
Dim: If you dim a light or if it dims, it becomes less bright.
Although it’s already well known
that we should pace our studies, new research suggests
that we should aim for “minimal interference” during these breaks –
deliberately avoiding any activity that could tamper with the delicate task of
memory formation. So no running errands, checking your emails, or surfing the
web on your smartphone. You really need to give your brain the chance for a
complete recharge with no distractions.
Pace: to walk with slow or regular steps
An excuse to do nothing may seem
like a perfect mnemonic technique for the lazy student, but this discovery may
also offer some relief for people with amnesia and some forms of dementia,
suggesting new ways to release a latent, previously unrecognised, capacity to
learn and remember.
The remarkable memory-boosting
benefits of undisturbed rest were first documented in 1900 by the German
psychologist Georg Elias Muller and his student Alfons Pilzecker. In one of
their many experiments on memory consolidation, Muller and Pilzecker first
asked their participants to learn a list of meaningless syllables. Following a
short study period, half the group were immediately given a second list to
learn – while the rest were given a six-minute break before continuing.
When tested one-and-a-half-hours
later, the two groups showed strikingly different patterns of recall. The
participants given the break remembered nearly 50% of their list, compared to
an average of 28% for the group who had been given no time to recharge their
mental batteries. The finding suggested that our memory for new information is
especially fragile just after it has first been encoded, making it more
susceptible to interference from new information.
Although a handful of other
psychologists occasionally returned to the finding, it was only in the early
2000s that the broader implications of it started to
become known, with a pioneering study by Sergio Della Sala at the University of
Edinburgh and Nelson Cowan at the University of Missouri.
Broader: Something that is broad is wide
The team was interested in
discovering whether reduced interference might improve the memories of people
who had suffered a neurological injury, such as a stroke. Using a similar
set-up to Muller and Pilzecker’s original study, they presented their
participants with lists of 15 words and tested them 10 minutes later. In some
trials, the participants remained busy with some standard cognitive tests; in
others, they were asked to lie in a darkened room and avoid falling asleep.
The impact of the small intervention
was more profound than anyone might have believed. Although the two most
severely amnesic patients showed no benefit, the others tripled the number of
words they could remember – from 14% to 49%, placing them almost within the
range of healthy people with no neurological damage.
The next results were even more
impressive. The participants were asked to listen to some stories and answer
questions an hour later. Without the chance to rest, they could recall just 7%
of the facts in the story; with the rest, this jumped to 79% – an astronomical
11-fold increase in the information they retained. The researchers also found a
similar, though less pronounced, benefit for healthy participants in each case,
boosting recall between 10 and 30%.
Della Sala and Cowan’s former
student, Michaela Dewar at Heriot-Watt University, has now led several
follow-up studies, replicating the finding in many different contexts. In
healthy participants, they have found that these short periods of rest can also
improve our spatial memories, for instance – helping participants to recall the
location of different landmarks in a virtual reality environment. Crucially, this advantage lingers
a week after the original learning task, and it seems to benefit young and old
people alike. And besides the stroke survivors,
they have also found similar benefits for people in the earlier, milder stages
of Alzheimer’s disease.
Crucially: Remarkably
Linger: Remain
Stroke: Apoplexy;
rupture of a blood vessel in the brain resulting in loss of consciousness,
often followed by paralysis, or embolism or thrombosis affecting a cerebral
vessel
In each case, the researchers simply
asked the participants to sit in a dim, quiet room, without their mobile phones
or similar distractions. “We don’t give them any specific instructions with
regards to what they should or shouldn’t do while resting,” Dewar says. “But
questionnaires completed at the end of our experiments suggest that most people
simply let their minds wander.”
Even then, we should be careful not
to exert ourselves too hard as we daydream. In
one study, for instance, participants were asked to imagine a past or future
event during their break, which appeared to reduce their later recall of the
newly learnt material. So it may be safest to avoid any concerted mental effort
during our down time.
Exert: To apply (oneself) with great energy or straining effort
The exact mechanism is still
unknown, though some clues come from a growing understanding of memory
formation. It is now well accepted that once memories are initially encoded,
they pass through a period of consolidation that cements them in long-term
storage. This was once thought to happen primarily during sleep, with
heightened communication between the hippocampus – where memories are first
formed – and the cortex, a process that may build and strengthen the new neural
connections that are necessary for later recall.
This heightened
nocturnal activity may be the reason that we often learn things better just
before bed. But in line with Dewar’s work, a 2010 study by Lila Davachi at New
York University, found that it was not limited to sleep, and similar neural
activity occurs during periods of wakeful rest, too. In the study, participants
were first asked to memorise pairs of pictures – matching a face to an object
or scene – and then allowed to lie back and let their minds wander for a short
period. Sure enough, she found increased communication between the hippocampus
and areas of the visual cortex during their rest. Crucially, people who showed
a greater increase in connectivity between these areas were the ones who
remembered more of the task, she says.
Heightened: If something heightens a feeling or if the feeling heightens, the
feeling increases in degree or intensity.
Perhaps the brain takes any
potential down time to cement what it has recently learnt – and reducing extra
stimulation at this time may ease that process. It would seem that neurological
damage may render the brain especially vulnerable to that interference after
learning a new memory, which is why the period of rest proved to be particularly
potent for stroke survivors and people with Alzheimer’s disease.
Other psychologists are excited
about the research. “The effect is quite consistent across studies now in a
range of experiments and memory tasks,” says Aidan Horner at the University of
York. “It’s fascinating.” Horner agrees that it could potentially offer new
ways to help individuals with impairments to function.
Practically speaking, he points out
that it may be difficult to schedule enough periods of rest to increase their
overall daily recall. But he thinks it could still be valuable to help a
patient learn important new information – such as learning the name and face of
a new carer. “Perhaps a short period of wakeful rest
after that would increase the chances that they would remember that person, and
therefore feel more comfortable with them later on.” Dewar tells me that she is
aware of one patient who seems to have benefitted from using a short rest to
learn the name of their grandchild, though she emphasises that it is only
anecdotal evidence.
Carer: A carer is someone who is responsible for looking after another
person, for example, a person who has a disability, or is ill or very young.
Thomas Baguley at Nottingham Trent
University in the UK is also cautiously optimistic. He points out that some
Alzheimer’s patients are already advised to engage in mindfulness techniques to
alleviate stress and improve overall well-being. “Some [of these] interventions
may also promote wakeful rest and it is worth exploring whether they work in
part because of reducing interference,” he says, though it may be difficult to
implement in people with severe dementia, he says.
Beyond the clinical benefits for
these patients, Baguley and Horner both agree that scheduling regular periods
of rest, without distraction, could help us all hold onto new material a little
more firmly. After all, for many students, the 10-30% improvements recorded in
these studies could mark the difference between a grade or two. “I can imagine
you could embed these 10-15 minute breaks within a revision period,” says
Horner, “and that might be a useful way of making small improvements to your
ability to remember later on.”
In the age of information overload,
it’s worth remembering that our smartphones aren’t the only thing that needs a
regular recharge. Our minds clearly do too.
Towards total recall
If you are interested in further,
low-effort ways to boost your recall, you may benefit from the following
strategies:
·
Test
yourself. So-called “retrieval practice” – actively forcing yourself to
remember information – is far more effective than passive reading.
·
“Space”
your studies, leaving a few weeks between the times you revisit material.
Indeed, it’s often better to wait until you are on the cusp of forgetting the
material to avoid “overlearning”.
·
Talk
to yourself. Simple describing an event cements it in your memory.
·
Add
variety. It can sometimes be beneficial to mix up and rotate the subjects you
are studying, a process called “interleaving”, rather than studying each one in
a single block.
This is one of the most significant information.
ResponEliminaI’m glad reading your article.
The site style is wonderful, the article is really excellent.
For more amazing information You may get from this link Recall The Past